The autistic spectrum: From theory to practice
The autistic spectrum: From theory to practice

This free course is available to start right now. Review the full course description and key learning outcomes and create an account and enrol if you want a free statement of participation.

Free course

The autistic spectrum: From theory to practice

4 Explaining autistic conditions: the socio-cognitive level

4.1 Do people with autistic spectrum disorders lack a theory of mind?

An important challenge for psychologists working on autistic spectrum disorders is to explain why characteristic impairments occur in three different areas of functioning (social interaction, communication, activities and interests). One strategy for explaining this three-way pattern is to identify a single underlying problem that links the different symptoms. Section 4 considers such ‘core deficit’ approaches. They all address autism at the socio-cognitive level, but make different assumptions about how cognitive and social functioning interrelate.

The most influential, and perhaps the most compelling, socio-cognitive model argues that people with ASDs have a diminished capacity to understand the thoughts, beliefs, intentions and emotions of other people, and perhaps themselves. Indeed, people with autism may be unaware that others have such a ‘mental life’. It has been argued that such theory of mind (ToM) was fundamental to the evolution of our species as advanced social animals. Making a similar point from a philosophical perspective, Daniel Dennett (1978) suggested that if a person was unable to understand the thoughts or intentions of another person, much of social interaction and communication would be a mystery. Hence a ToM deficit could explain the autistic difficulties in both these areas. The following anecdote illustrates this point. An autistic child was asked by his teacher ‘Go and ask Mr Smith [another teacher] if he would like a cup of coffee’. The child went and found Mr Smith and delivered the question, but then came straight back without waiting for the reply: he did not realise that the intention of these communications was to find out whether Mr Smith wanted a drink. Most conversation is inherently ambiguous, and to make sense of it, we use context and behaviour to work out the intentions behind what people say. Sperber and Wilson's concept of relevance is essentially this.

Dennett reasoned that the most stringent test of ToM was whether a person could understand that someone else's belief about a situation was different from their own, and from reality – the so-called false belief test. Imagine the following scenario:

You and a friend drive to the shops in your car. You park your car in a particular street (Mount Street) and as you both have different shops to visit you arrange to meet back at the car in an hour's time. Shortly after parting from your friend, you realise you have left your wallet at home, so you take the car home to fetch it. When you get back to where you parked before, it is full up, so you have to park in a different street (Park Street). You know that when your friend goes to meet you she will assume that the car is where you originally parked it. Unless you can find her first, she will go to meet you there.

In this situation you understand that your friend's belief about the location of the car is false, and that she will act on the basis of this false belief. Developmental studies suggest that children typically develop similar understanding at the age of four. Simon Baron-Cohen and colleagues (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985) designed an experimental test of whether children with autism could understand false belief, called the ‘Sally-Anne Task’, described in Box 4.

Box 4: The Sally-Anne False Belief Task

The child sits at a table on which there are two dolls (Anne and Sally), each placed facing a lidded container (a basket and a square box). The experimenter names the dolls for the child, and then checks that the child has understood which is which. The experimenter enacts a scenario of hiding a marble in the basket using one doll (Sally) to ‘hide’ the marble with the other (Anne) looking on. Sally then ‘leaves the room’ and the marble is re-hidden in the box. Sally then returns and the experimenter asks the child three questions.

  1. ‘Where will Sally look for her marble?’ (belief question: the correct answer is ‘in the basket’)

  2. ‘Where is the marble really?’ (reality question: the correct answer is ‘in the box’)

  3. ‘Where was the marble in the beginning?’ (memory question: the correct answer is ‘in the basket’)

Three groups of children were tested (one at a time) on the task:

Experimental group 20 autistic children with an average age of 11 years 11 months
Control group 1 14 children with Down's Syndrome with an average age of 10 years 11 months
Control group 2 27 typically developing children with an average age of 4 years 5 months

The children with autism selected for the task had an average mental age of nine years three months, as tested on a non-verbal I.Q. test, and five years five months as tested on a verbal test. The participants were selected such that both these mental age scores would be higher than those of the children with Down's syndrome and of the typically developing children (whose mental age would approximate their chronological age).

The children in all groups answered the reality and memory questions correctly. Eighty-five per cent of the typically developing children and eighty-six per cent of the children with Down's syndrome also answered the belief question correctly. In contrast eighty per cent of the children with autism answered the belief question incorrectly. That is, when asked ‘Where will Sally look for her marble?’ they pointed to the marble's current location rather than to where the marble had been re-hidden in Sally's absence.

The Sally-Anne False Belief Task
Frith, U. (1989) Autism: Explaining the Enigma, Basil Blackwell Ltd ©
Frith, U. (1989) Autism: Explaining the Enigma, Basil Blackwell Ltd
Figure 4: The Sally-Anne False Belief Task
(Baron-Cohen et al., 1985)

Activity 4

Why do you think the experimenter had selected autistic children with a higher mental age than the other two groups of children? Why do you think there were two different control groups of participants?

Discussion

The higher mental age among the autistic participants was designed to ensure that the way they tackled the task was not simply due to lower intellectual level. The reason for including both typically developing children and children with Down's syndrome as control groups was to ensure that any differences in the experimental group are specifically associated with autism, and not with learning difficulties in general. Similar design features are common to many experimental studies of children with autism.

Why do autistic children fail on the ‘belief’ question in this experiment? They are intellectually as able as the other two groups of children. And they have no difficulty in remembering where the marble was originally hidden or in understanding where it had been relocated: they were able to answer both the memory question and the reality question correctly. One explanation is that since children with autism have difficulties with pretend play, they misunderstood the pretend ‘drama’ that was being ‘enacted’ by the dolls. But this was ruled out by later experiments that replicated the task using real people to enact the scenario.

Baron-Cohen et al. argued that autistic children tend to fail on the ‘belief’ question because, instead of ‘putting themselves in Sally's shoes’, they assume that her belief about where the marble is hidden is the same as their own knowledge of where the marble really is. In short, the study appears to show that children with autism lack the capacity for understanding another person's mental state. This now classic study stimulated a massive wave of research that has examined and refined the ToM hypothesis and tested its predictions. Autistic children have been found to have difficulties in many areas that are consistent with a ToM deficit. For instance, children with ASDs make little use of ‘mental state’ language (words like ‘think’, ‘know’, ‘believe’, ‘feel’) in their speech. They have difficulty understanding or engaging in deception. Irony (saying ‘what great weather’ when its pouring with rain) and metaphor (‘that will keep the wolf from the door’) are lost on them. Problems like these make young people with autism very vulnerable to deception or exploitation.

However, 20 per cent of the children with autism pass experimental ToM tasks like the Sally-Anne test. It is also only possible to test participants who can understand task instructions, which precludes testing profoundly autistic participants. Finally, the approach addresses ongoing behaviour and skills in individuals diagnosed as autistic, but it does not explain the process of development that leads to these outcomes. We will consider how the ToM approach has been elaborated to address this developmental trajectory, before considering other approaches that complement or challenge it.

Definitions

Theory of mind/mind reading: A person's capacity to understand the thoughts, intentions, beliefs and feelings of others (and themselves), sometimes known as ‘mind reading’.

False belief test: Experimental test of theory of mind requiring a person to demonstrate understanding that another person's belief may be different from their own or from reality.

DSE232_1

Take your learning further

Making the decision to study can be a big step, which is why you'll want a trusted University. The Open University has over 40 years’ experience delivering flexible learning and 170,000 students are studying with us right now. Take a look at all Open University courses.

If you are new to university level study, find out more about the types of qualifications we offer, including our entry level Access courses and Certificates.

Not ready for University study then browse over 900 free courses on OpenLearn and sign up to our newsletter to hear about new free courses as they are released.

Every year, thousands of students decide to study with The Open University. With over 120 qualifications, we’ve got the right course for you.

Request an Open University prospectus