1.4 Theory of Mind and thinking literally
ToM is about understanding other people’s
Participants in the test were presented with stories like this one which contains an example of irony:
Ann’s mother has spent a long time cooking Ann’s favourite meal: fish and chips. But when she brings it in, Ann is watching TV, and she doesn’t even look up or say thank you. Ann’s mother is cross and says ‘Well that’s very nice isn’t it! That’s what I call politeness!’
The participants were asked:
Question 1: Is it true what Ann’s mother says?
Question 2: Why does Ann’s mother say this?
While autistic participants were able to identify that what Ann’s mother says is not true, most struggle to identify why she might say it, suggesting, for instance, that she was ‘having a joke’. A person who has difficulty in reading the meanings and intentions behind other people’s utterances may find all such expressions, interpreted literally, really puzzling or disconcerting. The consequences can sometimes be really profound.
In this extract, Wenn Lawson describes how, years ago when autism was less well known, his literal interpretation of questions from a psychiatrist led him to be misdiagnosed with schizophrenia (Lawson and Roth, 2011).
Transcript
In Week 2 you viewed two video clips [Tip: hold Ctrl and click a link to open it in a new tab. (Hide tip)] titled ‘Socially awkward’ and 'Misunderstanding', which you might like to watch again, considering how ToM difficulties could affect the behaviour of the young man in the clip.
Just how ToM and language skills are linked is debatable (de Villiers, 2000). Autistic people with pronounced language problems are more likely to fail false belief tasks, possibly suggesting that language difficulties cause ToM difficulties rather than the other way round. With much greater awareness of literal-mindedness these days, organisations like the National Autistic Society advocate clear, straightforward language for communicating with people in the autistic community.